Teiwa language
Teiwa (also referred to as Tewa)[3] is a Papuan language spoken on the Pantar island in eastern Indonesia. The island is the second largest in the Alor archipelago, lying just west of the largest island Alor.
Teiwa | |
---|---|
Native to | Indonesia |
Region | Pantar Island |
Native speakers | 4,000 (2010)[1] |
Dialects |
|
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | twe |
Glottolog | teiw1236 |
ELP | Teiwa [2] |
Teiwa Teiwa Teiwa | |
Coordinates: 8.38°S 124.17°E |
Teiwa is a morphosyntactically simple language with little inflection and is as such described as an isolating language, also known as an analytic language. It is pronounced by a complex pronoun system.
Nomenclature
Teiwa is also known as Bahasa Teiwa (the Teiwa language) in Indonesia. Teiwa itself is a nominal compound and can be translated as tei wa, meaning "tree leaf". The term "Teiwa" derives from the name of the main clan that speaks it. Generally, when Teiwa speakers refer to their own language, especially to differentiate it from the national language Indonesian, they call it "pitarau" (our language).
Classification
Teiwa is often classified as part of the Trans-New Guinea language family, but this is disputed. One reason is little lexical proof as well as the large geographical distance from the main island of New Guinea. An alternative classification is as part of the Timor-Alor-Pantar language family, which is approximately 3000 years old. Within this language family, Teiwa is further categorized within the sub-family of the Alor-Pantar languages, which are 20 in number. This classification bases on the high number of cognates as well as very similar pronoun systems.
Background
Teiwa is spoken on the island of Pantar, which is part of the Alor Archipelago, located between Australia and Indonesia. The island is located approximately 1000 km from the main island of New Guinea. It stretches 50 km from north to south, and between 11 and 29 km from east to west. The island is split into two distinct geographic regions: the dry and less populated lowlands in the west, and the highlands in the east, which are mountainous, volcanic and densely populated.
There were 4000 documented native speakers of Teiwa in 2010. The speakers live primarily in the desas (administrative villages in Indonesia) Lebang, Boweli, Kalib, Nule, Kadir, and Madar, a village of 460 inhabitants (as of 2007). Lebang is the main village, where Teiwa was still spoken by most people, young and old. Nevertheless, the national language of Indonesian as well as the Chinese-influenced Alor-Malay tend to be spoken by the younger generations and used for teaching in schools. As a result of this dwindling number of native speakers, Teiwa is listed as an endangered language.
The Grammar of Teiwa by Margaret Klamer is the only linguistic documentation besides a short word list from Stokhof (1975). Klamer gathered most of her data in the village of Madar.
Phonology
The following is a phonological description of Teiwa:[3]
Consonants
Teiwa has an inventory of 20 consonants, a high amount relative to other Papuan languages. In the table below, the orthographic representation of the sound is given in parentheses to the right. The contrast between the pharyngeal and glottal fricative shows itself as exceptional within the languages of Eastern Indonesia, as is the existence of both liquids /l/ and /r/.
Bilabial | Labiodental | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Uvular | Pharyngeal | Glottal | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Plosive (Stop/Oral Occlusive) | p b | t d | k ɡ | q (q) | ʔ (') | |||
Nasal | m | n | ŋ (ng) | |||||
Fricative | ɸ (f) | v | s | ħ (x) | h (h) | |||
Approximant (Glide) | w | j (y) | ||||||
Trill | r | |||||||
Lateral Approximant (Liquid) | l |
The allophones of /ɸ/ are [ɸ] and [p]. The allophones of /v/ are [v] and [f].
Vowels
Teiwa has an inventory of 5 cardinal vowels. The two high vowels occur as short (/i/, /u/) and long (/uː/, /iː/). As in the consonant table, the orthographic representations are given in the parentheses to the right.
Front | Back | |
---|---|---|
High/Close | /i/ /iː/ (ii) | /u/ /uː/ (uu) |
Mid | /ɛ/ (e) | /ɔ/ (o) |
Low/Open | /a/ (aa) | /ɑ/ (a) |
The allophones of /a/ are the short [a] and the long [aː].
Grammar
The grammar of Teiwa is as follows:[3]
Grammatical relations
Grammatical relations are the relations between argument and predicate. In Teiwa, these are formally expressed through pronouns from the object and subject paradigms, as well as a strict constituent order.
The subject relation is the agent argument of a transitive verb, from hereon denoted with A, or the single argument of an intransitive predicate, from hereon denoted with S. Both are encoded similarly.
The object relation is the non-agent argument of a transitive verb, from hereon denoted with P.
Basic constituent order
Teiwa is syntactically head-final, with Object-Verb constituent order: preverbal subject and object, sentence final verbs, negations, and conjunctions.
With intransitive verbs, there is SV-order. With transitive verbs, there is APV-order.
S | V | A | P | V |
---|---|---|---|---|
[...ki uwaad nuk] | yaa | ø | [bif ga'an] | tu'u |
eagle big one | descend | child 3s | knock | |
'...a big eagle came down [and] picked that child...' | ||||
Abbreviations: 3s = third person singular |
The A of the second (transitive) verb tu'uk coreferences with (shares the same reference as) the S of the first (intransitive) verb yaa in the example above.
A | P | V | S | V |
---|---|---|---|---|
...qau ba a | [a-sepatu qas] | usan ga-luxun-luxun ta | a | xer-an pati. |
good SEQ 3s | 3s-shoe(IND) split | lift 3s-RDP-high TOP | 3s | yell-REAL PROG |
'...so he lifts up one side of a shoe very high while he is yelling...' | ||||
Abbreviations: 3s = third person singular, SEQ = sequential marker, RDP = reduplication, TOP = topic (marked with ta), REAL = realis, PROG = progressive |
In this example, the Subject (A) is the pronominal, and the object (P) is the lexical NP (noun phrase).
Personal pronouns
There are three pronoun paradigms in Teiwa: subject, object, and possessive. The 'theme vowel' for singular pronouns is <a>, and for plural pronouns it is <i>. The second syllable of the long pronoun is a copy of the theme vowel with the addition of an -n.[3]
There is a contrast of inclusive-exclusive first person plural, one of the most prominent features to diffuse from the Austronesian languages into the Papuan languages.
Subject pronouns
Subject pronouns appear before the object and verb.
Long subject pronoun | Short subject pronoun | |
1s | na'an | na |
2s | ha'an | ha |
3s | a'an | a |
1p.exclusive | ni'in | ni |
1p.inclusive | pi'in | pi |
2p | yi'in | yi |
3p | iman | i, a |
3p.elsewhere | i'in | i, a |
distributive | ta'an | ta |
The long subject pronoun is used to set contrastive focus (me, not you), which can further be marked with la as the focus NP. They look nearly identical to the free object pronouns, save for the 3s and 3p.elsewhere pronouns.
Examples:
Na'an | hamar. | |
---|---|---|
1s.long | pray | |
'I pray [not you].' | ||
Na'an | la | hamar. |
1s.long | FOC | pray |
'I am the one who prays.' | ||
Abbreviations: 1s = first person singular |
The short subject pronoun is a "reduced pronoun" which can stand alone in place of nominal constituents, and is separable from the verb. Its paradigm is nearly identical to that of the object prefixes, except for the 3s, 3p, and 3p.elsewhere pronouns.
Examples:
Na | hamar. | |
---|---|---|
1s.short | pray | |
'I pray.' | ||
Na | g-oqai | ga-regan. |
1s.short | 3s.child | 3s.ask |
'I asked his child.' | ||
Abbreviations: 1s = first person singular |
Both the short and long object pronouns can express S and A.
Object pronouns
(free) Object pronoun | Object prefix | |
1s | na'an | n(a)- |
2s | ha'an | h(a)- |
3s | ga'an | g(a)-, gə- |
1p.exclusive | ni'in | n(i)- |
1p.inclusive | pi'in | p(i)- |
2p | yi'in | y(i), |
3p | iman | g(i)-, ga- |
3p.elsewhere | gi'in | g(i)- |
distributive | ta'an | t(a)- |
The underlined pronouns are a reminder of the differences to the long subject pronoun and short subject pronoun paradigms, respectively.
The object prefix has a consonantal and syllabic (in parentheses) form: the consonantal form appears before a verb beginning with a vowel, and the syllabic form appears before a verb beginning with a consonant.
The object pronoun is for both animate and inanimate referents, whereas the object prefix is exclusively for animate referents.
With the 3p (third person plural) object prefix, the differentiation of number is lost. In this case, number is specified through use of the additional pronoun ga'an (singular), iman (plural), or the plural word non in the object NP.
The 3s (third person singular) object pronoun maintains a further purpose as a demonstrative pronoun to introduce new participants into the discourse.
Possessive pronouns
Long pronoun | Short pronoun | Prefix | |
1s | na'an | na | n(a)- |
2s | ha'an | ha | h(a)- |
3s | a'an | a | g(a)-, a- |
1p.exclusive | ni'in | ni | n(i)- |
1p.inclusive | pi'in | pi | p(i)- |
2p | yi'in | yi | y(i), |
3p | iman | - | g(i)-, a-, ga- |
3p.elsewhere | gi'in | - | - |
distributive | ta'an | ta | t(a)- |
The final two pronouns, elsewhere and distributive, are unique. The 3p.elsewhere pronoun is used in a situation where the speaker cannot see the referent, because the referent is somewhere else.
For example:
I'in | g-oqai | ga-wei. |
---|---|---|
they.elsewhere | 3s.child | 3s.bathe |
'They (elsewhere) bathe/have bathed his child.' | ||
Abbreviations: 3s = third person singular |
Contrast this with the standard, unmarked form (3p):
Iman | g-oqai | ga-wei. |
---|---|---|
they | 3s.child | 3s.bathe |
'They bathe/have bathed his child.' | ||
Abbreviations: 3s = third person singular |
The distributive possessive pronoun (ta'an, ta, or ta-) refers to a (non-collective) plural number of human referents, often in reciprocal contexts.
Ta'an | tara' | mis! |
---|---|---|
DISTR | be.in.a.row | sit |
'Let's sit in a row!' (Lit. 'Each (one) sits in a row!') |
One more special possessive pronoun is li'in, which marks plurality of the possessor NP, and only as an adnominal modifier.[3]
Compare:
Uy | ga-yaf | |
---|---|---|
person | 3s.house | |
'Someone's house, a person's house' | ||
Uy | li'in | ga-yaf |
person | their | 3s.house |
'People's house(s)' |
Nouns
In Teiwa, the noun typically appears as head of the NP. The noun, with a few exceptions, cannot be reduplicated, unlike verbs, adverbs, and adjectives. (See Reduplication below.)
There is no marking for number, gender, or case on nouns. Instead, person and number is marked via a possessor prefix on the noun.
Noun phrases
In possessed NP's, the possessor (the noun which possesses) precedes the possessee (the noun which is possessed), as in the examples below.
Rai | ga-yaf |
king | 3s-house |
'The king's house.' | |
Abbreviations: 3s = third person singular |
Pi | pi-krian | i | er | a | gula' | sin. |
1pi | 1pi.work | PROX | make | 3s | finish | first |
'We first finish our work here.' | ||||||
Abbreviations: 1pi = first person plural inclusive, PROX = proximal |
In non-possessed NP's, the noun comes first, followed by the modifying element, such as an adjective.
Xaf | uwaad |
fish | big |
'A big fish.' |
Uy | a |
person | PROX |
'This person.' |
Noun classes
The Teiwa nouns can be divided into two main classes: Proper nouns and common nouns.
Proper nouns
Proper nouns are not modifiable. Examples are listed below.[3]
Male names: Edi, Goli, Lius, Mase, Nabas, Ribu
Female names: Bruang, Leti, Malai, Mani, Sam
Family names: Biri, Blegar, Bui, Lau, Qoli, Ribu, Unu
Clan names: Barawasi, Burilak, Loxoq, Perang Tubi, Qailipi
Common nouns
The common nouns can be further divided into subclasses:
Nouns with alienable possession
In this subclass the possessor prefix is optional. Focus can be placed with use of a long pronoun. Examples include: yaf 'house', kon 'shirt', qavif 'goat'.
Nouns with inalienable possession
Here the possessor prefix is obligatory, to the point that native speakers will not recognize the word without the prefix. Nouns with inalienable possession include body parts, and kinship terms (except for emaq 'wife' where the prefix is optional as with alienable possession).
Locational nouns
This last subclass of nouns denote location. Examples include: wanan 'side', fan 'front', siban 'behind', ragan 'outside', tag 'up(stairs); above speaker (relatively close)'.
Uy | ragan | me' |
---|---|---|
people | outside | be.in |
'Some people are outside' / 'Someone is outside' |
Nominalization
There is no dedicated morphology for nominalization in Teiwa. Instead the third person (3p) possessor prefix -ga has a secondary function of attaching to the root form of adjectives, locational nouns, adverbs, and question words.[3]
Verbs
Teiwa verbs carry no marking for case or gender. There is only one verbal suffix;
-(a)n for the marking of realis status. Only verbs take an object prefix. Inflected prefixes index person and number traits of animate objects on the verb. Subjects and inanimate objects are not indexed on the verb.
Teiwa has intransitive and transitive verbs. The transitive verbs are monotransitive, meaning they have a single grammatical object.
Transitive verbs
The transitive verbs in Teiwa can be divided into numerous sub classes, based on how they encode animate and inanimate objects differently. In this case, animate or inanimate refers explicitly to a third person referent, since first and second person referents are inherently animate.
- Class (i)
- Verbs with an object prefix, with an animate object ("sb-somebody")
This class expresses the object with an object-marking prefix on the verb. The prefix marks for person and number. The lexical NP is optional and may be used to clarify or disambiguate the referent.
Examples of verbs are: an ‘give sb’, ‘an ‘sell to sb’, ayas ‘throw at sb’, bun ‘answer sb’, fin ‘catch sb’, liin ‘invite sb’, regan ‘ask sb’, sas ‘feed sb’, walas ‘tell sb’, wei ‘bathe sb’
Examples of such verbs in sentence constructions:
A | qavif | ga-uyan | gi | si... |
---|---|---|---|---|
3s | goat | 3s.search | go | SIM |
'He went searching for [a] goat...' | ||||
Abbreviations: 3s = third person singular, SIM = simultaneous marker |
The prefix ga- on the verb -uyan marks for third person singular object, that is for qavif, 'goat'. Goat is an animate object.
A | yivar | ga-walas | a | wa... |
---|---|---|---|---|
3s | dog | 3s.tell | 3s | say |
'He told [his] dog...' | ||||
Abbreviations: 3s = third person singular |
Here similarly, the prefix ga- on the verb -walas marks for third person singular object, that is for yivar, 'dog'. Dog is an animate object.
- Class (ii)
- Verbs without an object prefix, with inanimate object ("sth-something")
Here the verb encodes the object as a separate nominal constituent. In this class the encoding with a prefix is disallowed.
Examples of such verbs are: bali ‘see sth’, ol ‘buy sth’, paai ‘cut sth in many small pieces’, put ‘cut off (grass)’ An example in a sentence construction:
...i'in | i-xaf | uwaad | la | boqai | dau-an | na. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
they.elsewhere | 3p.fish | big | FOC | cut.up | cook-REAL | eat |
'...they cut up their big fish, cooked and ate [it]' | ||||||
Abbreviations: 3p = third person plural, FOC = focus, REAL = realis |
The verbs in this sentence have no object prefix, and the object 'fish' is inanimate (because it is no longer living).
- Class (iii)
- Transitive verbs that take either animate or inanimate objects
iiia. Transitive verbs with prefixed animate object OR free (unfixed) inanimate object
With free inanimate object (object prefix not bound to verb).
Na | ga'an | mar. |
---|---|---|
1s | 3s | take |
'I take/get it.' | ||
Abbreviations: 3s = third person singular |
With prefixed animate object
Na | ga-mar. |
---|---|
1s | 3s-take |
'I follow him/her.' | |
Abbreviations: 1s = first person singular, 3s = third person singular |
Notice the important difference in meaning with the use a prefixed pronoun versus a free pronoun!
iiib. Verbs with an animate OR inanimate object, both as a prefix
Third person object prefixes marking animate or inanimate:
3sg inanimate object | 3sg animate object |
ga- | ga'- |
ge- | |
g- |
The contrasts are illustrated in the below translations:
wulul | 'speak, talk, tell' |
ga'-wulul | 'talk with sb, tell sb' |
ga-wulul | 'talk about sth, tells sth' |
A glottal stop is used for animate objects. The canonical form is used for inanimate objects.
Sound verbs
An interesting class of verbs constituting verbs for sounds made by animals or objects.[3]
aga-aga | sound to call a dog |
ago-ago | sound to call a dog (remote) |
sika | sound to chase away a dog |
sumax | sound to chase away a goat |
burax | sound to chase away chickens |
kuru-kuru | sound to call chickens |
xo' | to bark (dog) |
ox | to grunt (pig) |
qau | to scream (pig) |
hong | dog's sound ('woof') |
kokoko | chicken's sound ('tock-tock') |
quququ | 'cock-a-doodle-doo' |
me'eh | goat's sound |
paq | sound of a rock that is crushing corn |
qabunggat | splashing sound of rock in water |
tadunggat | dry sound of rock falling on land |
saxa' | flapping sound of something light falling (e.g. sandals on street) |
Experiencer predicates
These are predicates formed with the bodypart noun -om 'inside'[3]
n-om quun | 'I am smart/clever.' |
1s-inside be.sure | |
n-om qau | 'I am happy.' |
1s-inside-good | |
n-om siis | 'I am thirsty' |
1s-inside dry | |
n-om par | 'I am annoyed (at s.b.)' |
1s-inside defeated | |
n-om qalixil | 'I am angry' |
1s-inside itchy | |
n-om mai | 'I am planning/I plan' |
1s-inside store/keep | |
n-om bangan | 'I want/like' |
1s-inside see | |
n-om ga-regan | 'I think/say to myself' |
1s-inside 3s-ask | |
n-om ga-i' | 'I feel pity for him' |
1s-inside 3s-sick |
Reduplication
Reduplication is a morphological process to express greater intensity or the repeated/ongoing nature of an event. In Teiwa, the entire root is copied; there exists no productive process for syllable reduplication.[3]
- of verbs
tewar-tewar | 'walk on and on' |
RDP-walk | |
tib-tib | 'exactly enough' |
RDP-provide.for | |
haqax-haqax | 'take a few rests' |
RDP-breathe |
- of verbs with the realis suffix
Reduplication of the entire stem including the realis suffix takes place. Only verbs can be inflected for realis mood.
tii'in-tii'in | 'being asleep', 'sleep on and on' |
RDP-[sleep-REAL] | |
miran-mir-an | 'climb on and on', 'continue to climb' |
RDP-[ascend-REAL] | |
moxodan-moxod-an | 'let fall, drop down' |
RDP-[drop-REAL] |
- of adverbials
wek-wek | 'behind' |
RDP-behind | |
bas-bas* | 'usually' |
RDP-tomorrow | |
bes-bes | 'good morning' |
RDP-morning |
- of adjectives
musaq-musaq | 'very much shattered' |
RDP-shattered | |
[qa'an-qa'an]'an* | 'be black-REAL' > 'something black' |
[RDP-black-] v-REAL |
*The adjectival base is first turned into a verb through reduplication, in order to allow the realis suffix to be reduplicated (only verbs can be reduplicated with a realis suffix).
- of numerals
Iman | nuk-nuk | / | raq-raq | / | yerig-yerig | aria-n. |
they | RDP-one | RDP-two | RDP-three | arrive-REAL | ||
'They arrive one by one, two by two, three by three.' |
- of nouns
Reduplication of nouns is rarer, and does not serve to express plurality of distributivity.
mug-mug | 'be hilly' (something attributiv) |
RDP-mountain.top | |
war-war | 'day after day' (something adverbial) |
RDP-day |
Kinship
The Teiwa live in exogamous, patrilineal clans: the children belong to the clan of the father. The term "Teiwa" refers to a group of (sub) clans with the same ancestors. The Teiwa branch into two moieties (halves), which are separate genealogical supergroups, each of which includes multiple clans.
Teiwa | |
---|---|
I. | II. |
Baraqala | Lambar |
La Builan | Kakalau |
Salanggalu | Lau Wad |
Maligi | Loxog |
Hukung | Kaloman Goqar |
Qailipi |
Children are named with 1) clan name, 2) given name 3) father's family name, for example Teiwa Jance Wa'ng.
Kinship system
The kinship system of the Teiwa is based on cross-cousins. This means that the children of same-sex siblings are considered to be siblings (brother, sister), and therefore not fit for marriage with one another. Children of non-same sex siblings of the parents are seen as cross-cousins and are the perfect candidates for marriage with each other. These children are also in a different clan than the children of the same-sex siblings of the parents.
Kinship terms
The main kinship terms are listed here:
emaq | wife |
misi | husband |
bif | child, 'younger sibling' |
biar (kriman) | children |
na-gas qai | my sister |
n-ian qai | my brother |
n-ian | (female) cousin of ego, in other clan |
na-dias | (male) cousin of ego, in other clan |
na-rat (emaq) | daughter of ego's brother, in other clan (potential daughter-in-law) |
na'ii | son of ego's brother, in other clan (potential son-in-law) |
na-rata' | my grandfather/mother |
na-rat qai | my grandchild |
From the point of view of female ego:
The "classificatory siblings" refer to the actual siblings, as well as the children of the mother's sister and the father's brother. As it is considered rude to call family members by their given name, these siblings are addressed as matu' when older and bif when younger, and ka'au when the same sex as the speaker.
The "classificatory parents" are the father's brother (n-oma 'my father'), as well as the mother's sister (na-xala 'my mother'). Each person therefore has two sets of parents.
References
- Teiwa at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015)
- Endangered Languages Project data for Teiwa.
- Klamer 2010, pp. 166
Further reading
- Klamer, Marian (2010). A Grammar of Teiwa. Berlin: De Gruyter.CS1 maint: ref=harv (link)
- Klamer, Marian (2017) [first published 2014]. "The Alor-Pantar languages: Linguistic context, history and typology". In Klamer, Marian (ed.). The Alor-Pantar languages: History and Typology. Studies in Diversity Linguistics (second ed.). Berlin: Language Science Press. doi:10.5281/zenodo.437098.CS1 maint: ref=harv (link)
External links
For a list of words relating to Teiwa language, see the Teiwa language category of words in Wiktionary, the free dictionary. |