Vision span

Vision span or perceptual span is a controversial concept referring to the angular span (vertically and horizontally), within which the human eye has sharp enough vision to perform an action accurately (reading or face recognition). The visual field of the human eye spans approximately 120 degrees of arc.[1] However, most of that arc is peripheral vision. The human eye has much greater resolution in the macula, where there is a higher density of cone cells. The field of view that is observed with sufficient resolution to read text typically spans about 6 degrees of arc, which is wide enough to allow a clear view of about five words in a row when printed text at ordinary size is held about 50 centimeters from the eyes. Regarding face processing, the field of view with a sufficient amount of information in order to recognise faces accurately spans about 7° which represents about 45% of a face.[2] The brain creates the illusion of having a greater visual span by automatically and unconsciously moving the center of vision into any area of interest in the field of view.

Peripheral vision of the human eye.

Application to speed reading

While reading, readers will fail to recognize a word unless they are fixating within three to four character spaces of the word.[3] The same is true for speed readers and skimmers. Speed readers cannot answer questions about a main point or detail, if they did not fixate directly on it or within three character spaces of it. [4] When a text is removed whilst reading, readers can only accurately report upon the word they were fixating upon or the next one to the right.[5] There is no evidence from eye movement research that individuals are making predictions of text based upon hypotheses about the words in the periphery so that they can skip over or spend less time on unimportant or redundant words.[3]

Most speed reading courses claim that the peripheral vision can be used to read text. This has been suggested impossible because the text is blurred out through lack of visual resolution. At best the human brain can only guess at the content of text outside the macular region. There simply are not enough cone cells away from the center of the visual field to identify words in the periphery of the field.

It has been suggested that the fixation span can be stretched through training (meta guiding) to take in as much as a line for the purpose of skimming or speed reading.[6] However other sources suggest that using this method can result in a severely reduced comprehension rate in comparison to normal reading ("rauding").

Some speed reading courses stress that the human eye has to move very quickly. They also stress that the human eye should move in a pattern to fill in the information that was not properly perceived. The effective limit for scanning speeds based upon the limit of the human eye's resolution is about 300 words per minute.[7] It is claimed that such speeds also require great practice, and extremely rapid eye movements, although research suggests that such training is not possible. It has been suggested by some speed reading promoters that the readers who achieve such speeds are on the autism spectrum. Research into reading rate suggests that study strategies, rather than speed reading, explains why expert readers, such as professors and editors, are more efficient than others.

Application to face processing

Face recognition system requires several fixation on different location (typically in a triangular pattern) for a face to be recognised. The same is true for the so-called Super Recognisers who are persons with high face recognition capabilities. Similarly to the reading span, the Facespan might be modulated by difficulty,[8] expertise,[9] age,[10] disorder [11] and other idiosyncratic differences between individuals. The idiosyncratic differences are thus most probably due to differences in Facespan rather than in oculomotor strategy. Previous study revealed that the Vision span is more accurately defined in number of characters rather than in visual angle.[12]

References

  1. Hammoud, Riad (2008). Hammoud, Riad I (ed.). Passive Eye Monitoring: Algorithms, Applications and Experiments. Signals and Commmunication Technologies. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-75412-1. ISBN 978-3-540-75411-4.
  2. Papinutto, Michael; Lao, Junpeng; Ramon, Meike; Caldara, Roberto; Miellet, Sébastien (2017). "The Facespan—the perceptual span for face recognition". Journal of Vision. 17 (5): 16. doi:10.1167/17.5.16. PMID 28549354.
  3. Rayner, Keith (1975). "The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading". Cognitive Psychology. 7 (4): 65–81. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(75)90005-5. S2CID 54366006.
  4. Just and Carpenter 1987
  5. McConkie and Hogaoam 1985
  6. Legge, G. E; Cheung, S. H; Yu, D; Chung, S. T; Lee, H. W; Owens, D. P (2007). "The case for the visual span as a sensory bottleneck in reading". Journal of Vision. 7 (2): 9.1–915. doi:10.1167/7.2.9. PMC 2729064. PMID 18217824.
  7. Caroll, Robert Todd (26 October 2015). "Speed-reading". The Skeptic's Dictionary.
  8. Henderson, John M.; Ferreira, Fernanda (1990). "Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: Implications for attention and eye movement control". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 16 (3): 417–429. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.417. PMID 2140401. S2CID 1988041.
  9. Rayner, Keith; Slattery, Timothy J; Bélanger, Nathalie N (2010). "Eye movements, the perceptual span, and reading speed". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 17 (6): 834–9. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.6.834. PMC 3075059. PMID 21169577.
  10. Rayner, Keith (1986). "Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 41 (2): 211–36. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(86)90037-8. PMID 3701249.
  11. Rayner, Keith (1983). "Eye movements, perceptual span, and reading disability". Annals of Dyslexia. 33: 163–173. doi:10.1007/BF02648003. S2CID 143088257.
  12. Miellet, Sébastien; O'Donnell, Patrick J; Sereno, Sara C (2009). "Parafoveal Magnification" (PDF). Psychological Science. 20 (6): 721–8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02364.x. PMID 19470124. S2CID 8476978.

Further reading

  • McConkie and Hogaoam 1985. Eye position and word identification during reading. In R. Groner et al.'s Eye movements and information processing. Amsterdam, Elsevier.
  • Just and Carpenter 1987. In Allyn & Bacon's The Psychology of Reading and Language Comprehension. Boston.

See also

This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.