Live and let live (World War I)
Live and let live is the spontaneous rise of non-aggressive co-operative behaviour that developed during the First World War, particularly during prolonged periods of trench warfare on the Western Front. Perhaps one of the most famous examples of this is the Christmas Truce of 1914.
It is a process that can be characterised as the deliberate abstaining from the use of violence during war. Sometimes it can take the form of overt truces or pacts negotiated locally by soldiers. At other times it can be a tacit behaviour—sometimes characterised as "letting sleeping dogs lie"—whereby both sides refrain from firing or using their weapons, or deliberately discharge them in a ritualistic or routine way that signals their non-lethal intent.
Examples
This behaviour was found at the small-unit level, sections, platoons or companies, usually observed by the "other ranks", e.g., privates and non-commissioned officers. Examples were found from the lone soldier standing sentry duty, refusing to fire on exposed enemy soldiers, up to snipers, machine-guns teams and even field-artillery batteries.
Upper echelon commanders—those of divisions, corps and armies—and their staffs were aware of this tendency towards non-aggression, and would sometimes analyse casualty statistics to detect it. Raids or patrols were often ordered to foster the correct "offensive spirit" in the troops.[1]
The Live and Let Live system was fragile at best and was easily broken by occurrences of lethal force, becoming even more tenuous as the war dragged on.
Research
Tony Ashworth[2] researched this topic based upon diaries, letters, and testimonies of veterans from the war. He discovered that 'live and let live' was widely known about at the time and was most common at specific times and places. It was often to be found when a unit had been withdrawn from battle and was sent to a rest sector.
Game theory
Some scholars of game theory, like Robert Axelrod, have characterised Live and Let Live as an iterated variant of the prisoner's dilemma. Axelrod linked Live and Let Live to the co-operative strategy referred to as Tit for Tat.
Axelrod's interpretation of "Live and Let Live" as a prisoner's dilemma has been disputed by political scientists Joanne Gowa[3] and Andrew Gelman,[4] who (separately) argue that the assumptions underlying the prisoner's dilemma do not hold in this example.
References
- Kellett, Anthony (1982). Combat Motivation: The Behaviour of Soldiers in Battle. Dordrecht: Springer Science+Business Media. p. 147. ISBN 978-0-89838-106-1.
- Ashworth, Tony (2000). Trench Warfare 1914–1918: The Live and Let Live System. London: Pan. ISBN 0330480685.
- Anarchy, Egoism, and Third Images: The Evolution of Cooperation and International Relations The Evolution of Cooperation. by Robert Axelrod Review by: Joanne Gowa. International Organization, Vol. 40, No. 1 (Winter, 1986), pp. 167-186. https://www.jstor.org/stable/pdfplus/2706746.pdf
- Andrew Gelman, Game theory as ideology: some comments on Robert Axelrod's The Evolution of Cooperation. QA-Rivista dell'Associazione Rossi-Doria 2, 167-176 (1988). http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/research/published/gelmanaxelrod_001.pdf
Further reading
- Ashworth, Tony. (1968). "The Sociology of Trench Warfare", British Journal of Sociology, 21:407–20.
- Axelrod, Robert. (2006). The Evolution of Cooperation, Revised edition. Perseus Books Group, ISBN 0465005640
- Sheffield, G. D. (2000). Leadership in the Trenches: Officer-Man Relations, Morale and Discipline in the British Army in the Era of the First World War. Basingstoke: Macmillan. ISBN 0333654110
- Collaboration. (2007) Meetings in No Man's Land. Constable & Robinson. ISBN 1845295137
- Dawkins, Richard. (1976). The Selfish Gene 30th Anniversary Edition Oxford. ISBN 978-0-19-929115-1